SOLUTION: Carnegie Mellon University What Is Meant by The Term Phenological Mean Questions

SOLUTION: Carnegie Mellon University What Is Meant by The Term Phenological Mean Questions.

Ecology, 100(11), 2019, e02826
© 2019 by the Ecological Society of America
Shifts in phenological mean and synchrony interact to shape
competitive outcomes
SHANNON K. CARTER
1
AND VOLKER
H. W. RUDOLF
Department of Biosciences, Program in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Rice University, 6100 Main Street, MS-170, Houston,
Texas 77005-1892 USA
Citation: Carter, S. K., and V. H. W. Rudolf. 2019. Shifts in phenological mean and synchrony
interact to shape competitive outcomes. Ecology 100(11):e02826. 10.1002/ecy.2826
Abstract. Climate change–induced phenological shifts are ubiquitous and have the potential to disrupt natural communities by changing the timing of species interactions. Shifts in first
and/or mean phenological date are well documented, but recent studies indicate that shifts in
synchrony (individual variation around these metrics) can be just as common. However, we
know little about how both types of phenological shifts interact to affect species interactions
and communities. Here, we experimentally manipulated the hatching phenologies of two competing species of larval amphibians to address this conceptual gap. Specifically, we manipulated
the relative mean hatching time (early, same, or late relative to competitor) and population synchrony (high, medium, or low levels of variation around the mean) in a full 3 9 3 factorial
design to measure independent and interactive effects of phenological mean and population
phenological synchrony on competitive outcomes. Our results indicate that phenological synchrony within a population strongly influences intraspecific competition by changing the density of individuals and relative strength of early- vs. late-arriving individuals. Individuals from
high-synchrony populations competed symmetrically, whereas individuals from low-synchrony
populations competed asymmetrically. At the community scale, shifts in population phenological synchrony interact with shifts in phenological mean to affect key demographic rates (survival, biomass export, per capita mass, and emergence timing) strongly. Furthermore, changes
in mean timing of species interactions altered phenological synchrony within a population at
the next life stage, and phenological synchrony at one life stage altered the mean timing of the
next life stage. Thus, shifts in phenological synchrony within populations cannot only alter
species interactions, but species interactions in turn can also drive shifts in phenology.
Key words: amphibians; competition; phenological shifts; phenology; species interactions; synchrony; timing.
INTRODUCTION
Phenologies, the seasonal timing of life-history events,
play an important role in driving the dynamics of natural
systems because they determine when an individual enters
an environment, and in turn, the individual’s stage and
size when interacting with other members of the community (Yang and Rudolf 2010, Thackeray et al. 2016).
Phenological shifts in spring life-history events are a common response to climate change across diverse taxa, and
it is a major goal to understand how these temporal shifts
will impact species interactions in natural communities
(Parmesan and Yohe 2003, Root et al. 2003, Menzel et al.
2006). To address this issue, previous research has predominantly focused on measuring shifts in the first or
mean occurrence of a phenological event in natural populations. These studies have consistently found that, across
varied taxa, the timing of first and mean spring lifeManuscript received 11 March 2019; revised 24 May 2019;
accepted 13 June 2019. Corresponding Editor: Kirk Winemiller.
1
E-mail: shannon.k.carter@rice.edu
history events advance in time to match the earlier onset
of spring due to climate change (Parmesan 2007, Taylor
2008, Vitasse et al. 2018). Because different species vary
in the magnitude of phenological response, interacting
species frequently become mismatched in time, which can
change interaction strength and disrupt natural communities (Renner and Zohner 2018, Rudolf 2018, Rudolf
and McCrory 2018). However, individuals within a species vary in their timing, creating a distribution of phenologies for a given life-history event at the population
level (hereafter phenological synchrony; Miller-Rushing
et al. 2010, Rasmussen and Rudolf 2015). Importantly,
the shape of this phenological distribution can change
among years and is closely tied to changing weather patterns, including climate change (Wolkovich et al. 2014,
Carter et al. 2018). As a result, shifts in a population’s
phenological synchrony can occur with equal or greater
frequency relative to shifts in first or mean phenological
events (CaraDonna et al. 2014, Carter et al. 2018), but
the consequences of shifts in synchrony for species interactions and regulation of communities remain poorly
understood.
Article e02826; page 1
Article e02826; page 2
SHANNON K. CARTER AND VOLKER H. W. RUDOLF
The importance of phenological synchrony for the regulation of natural populations becomes apparent when
we consider how synchrony affects both the density of
interacting individuals and per capita interaction
strength. Increasing the synchrony of a phenological event
within a population increases the average density of interacting individuals (Loe et al. 2005, Koenig et al. 2015).
Although this numerical effect should increase intraspecific competition, phenological synchrony can also alter
how much per-capita effects vary among individuals, that
is, competitive symmetry (Rudolf and Rasmussen 2013,
Rasmussen and Rudolf 2015). Offspring that hatch at the
same time will have similar sizes and thus have similar
(symmetric) competitive abilities, whereas offspring that
hatch earlier are typically competitively dominant over
smaller conspecifics that hatch later (Rudolf and Singh
2013, Rasmussen et al. 2014). Therefore, a low-synchrony
population should result in a low-density population
where individuals compete asymmetrically, whereas a
high-synchrony population should result in a high-density
population where individuals compete symmetrically
(Henson and Cushing 1996).
The picture is further complicated when we consider
the role of population phenological synchrony in a community context. Research on priority effects provides a
Ecology, Vol. 100, No. 11
strong foundation for understanding how relative mean
phenological events affect species interactions (Tilman
1988, Fukami 2010), but little is known about the role of
synchrony, or how these two aspects of phenology might
interact (Fig. 1, Rasmussen and Rudolf 2016). Considering two competing species, at least three major outcomes
are possible. First, there may be no effect of synchrony.
Effects of population synchrony may be overwhelmed by
stronger effects of relative mean arrival (i.e., an early
arriver benefits from priority access to the resource). In
this case, mean phenological events of populations are
sufficient to predict outcomes, meaning synchrony can
be ignored and outcomes across any column in Fig. 1
would be identical. Second, mean and synchrony may
have additive effects. Previous work has shown higher
survival for high-synchrony populations relative to lowsynchrony populations (Rasmussen and Rudolf 2015). If
the effects of mean and synchrony are additive, we would
then expect to see higher survival of high-synchrony
populations across a range of relative arrival times (e.g.,
survival of the orange population would increase moving down columns and across rows to the left in Fig. 1).
Finally, synchrony and mean might have interactive
effects on competitive outcomes. Phenological synchrony affects the proportion of individuals experiencing
Phenological mean of focal species
(relative to secondary species)
Same
Late
Medium
High
Phenological synchrony of focal species
Low
Number of individuals hatching
Early
Date
FIG. 1. Conceptual illustration of potential phenological shifts between two interacting species. For simplicity, we have designated a secondary species (represented in teal, Rana in our experiment) and adjusted the phenology of the focal species (represented
in orange, Hyla in our experiment) relative to it. Columns show differences in phenological mean between the two species and rows
show differences in phenological synchrony of the focal species. With concomitant shifts in phenological mean and synchrony, it is
difficult to intuit net effects on population demography and species interactions.
November 2019
EFFECTS OF PHENOLOGY ON COMPETITION
different conditions (compare top and bottom rows of
Fig. 1). In a high-synchrony population, all individuals
experience the same conditions, which could be good
(e.g., early arrival relative to competitor as shown in
Fig. 1, or alternatively, favorable environmental conditions) or bad (e.g., late arrival relative to competitor, or
harsh environmental conditions). Alternatively, low-synchrony populations spread individuals across good and
bad conditions, akin to bet hedging (Wilbur et al. 2006,
Tarazona et al. 2017, Rocha et al. 2018, Shima et al.
2018). Therefore, differences in synchrony could strongly
affect the outcome of shifts in mean phenology—a highsynchrony population might be more sensitive to shifts
in phenological mean because all individuals shift to
experience a new condition, whereas low synchrony
might be more robust to shifts in phenological mean.
Because the interplay between phenological mean and
phenological synchrony is difficult to intuit, we need
empirical data to give expectations for how these concomitant types of shifts are likely to affect natural systems.
The interaction between relative mean difference in
phenologies and population phenological synchrony
also has the potential to carry over to affect synchrony for subsequent phenological events. In the
absence of interspecific competition, per capita differences among individuals in low synchrony populations
should result in higher survival of the earliest individuals if competition is strong (Rasmussen et al. 2014),
potentially skewing the distribution of the next phenological stage to be clustered around an early event.
However, if competition is low, or if individuals are
not plastic in their development rates, a population’s
synchrony may be maintained from one phenological
stage to the next. By altering the density and/or size
differences among individuals within a population,
shifts in mean phenologies of an interspecific competitor can therefore also modify the intraspecific drivers
that increase or decrease phenological synchrony in a
focal population. If true, this would imply that shifts
in timing of interspecific interactions could be an
important but overlooked driver of variation in phenological synchrony of later life-history events, but this
remains to be tested.
Here, we evaluate the effects of phenological shifts
in a community context. Specifically, we use a mesocosm experiment to examine how different metrics
(mean and synchrony) of phenology affect the outcome of competition between two competing amphibian species. Specifically, we altered the order of arrival
(i.e., mean hatching date) between the two species and
the phenological synchrony of one of the two species.
This system allowed us to ask: (1) What are the independent and interactive effects of phenological mean
and phenological synchrony on population demography and competitive interactions? (2) Does a population’s
phenological
synchrony
change
across
phenological stages?
Article e02826; page 3
METHODS
Study system
We studied the gray tree frog (Hyla versicolor or Dryophytes versicolor, hereafter Hyla) and its competitor the
Southern Leopard frog (Rana sphenocephala or Lithobates sphenocephalus, hereafter Rana) to determine
effects of mean and synchrony of hatching phenology on
the performance of Hyla. We chose Hyla as the focal
species because it develops more quickly, enabling us to
capture the full period of its emergence and therefore
track phenological synchrony across phenological stages.
The two species are an ideal system for several reasons.
First, they commonly co-occur throughout the southeastern United States and are resource competitors, both
in larval and adult stages (Alford and Wilbur 1985). Second, both species show significant variation in the duration and seasonal timing of breeding (Carter et al.
2018), so we expect larval offspring to overlap at different times based on year-specific weather conditions.
Third, we are able to delay egg hatching in both species,
allowing us to manipulate phenology experimentally.
Finally, amphibians exhibit a strong but highly variable
phenological response relative to other taxa (Parmesan
2006, Todd et al. 2010) and are declining globally (Bury
1999, Stuart et al. 2004), suggesting they should be a
high priority for examining consequences of phenological shifts.
Experimental system and design
Egg clutches of Hyla and Rana were collected from
Davy Crockett National Forest on 30 March 2018. Initially, all clutches were maintained at 15°C to slow development. Then, 1–2 d prior to introduction to the
experiment, batches of eggs were moved to warmer conditions (25°C) to induce hatching. This allowed us to
introduce tadpole hatchlings of the same size (Gosner
stage 25; ~2.1 mm snout-to-vent length (SVL) for Hyla
and ~4.4 mm SVL for Rana) on different days. These
temperatures are well within the range both species
would experience in ephemeral ponds in nature, and
developmental assays have shown few negative side
effects on performance for tadpoles reared at these temperatures (Moore 1939, Rudolf and Singh 2013, Rasmussen and Rudolf 2016). The experiment was a full 3
(phenological synchrony) 9 3 (phenological mean) factorial design. In addition, we had single-species controls
manipulating synchrony only, which allowed us to separate the intraspecific effects of synchrony from the competitive effects of arrival order. To create our
phenological synchrony treatments, we manipulated the
variation in hatching date for Hyla around a mean
hatching date, 15 April 2018. For high-synchrony treatments, all 45 Hyla individuals hatched on 15 April. For
medium-synchrony treatments, hatching occurred on
3 d from 12 April–18 April. For low-synchrony
Article e02826; page 4
SHANNON K. CARTER AND VOLKER H. W. RUDOLF
treatments, hatching occurred on 5 d from 9 April 9–21
April. For medium- and low-synchrony treatments, the
45 Hyla individuals were equally divided among the
three and five introductions, respectively. To create
the phenological mean treatments, we manipulated
the hatching date of Rana to occur early (9 April), at the
same time (15 April), or late (21 April) relative to the
mean hatching date of Hyla. All Rana individuals for a
given treatment hatched on a single day (conceptual
schematic of treatments in Fig. 1 and detailed schedule
in Appendix S1: Fig. S1). Control (i.e., no interspecific
competition) populations lacked Rana. For both species,
a subset of individuals was photographed and measured
before each introduction, which confirmed that individual body sizes (for a given species) were the same across
all introductions (Appendix S1: Fig. S2). There were six
replicates per competition treatment and two replicates
of control populations, for a total of 60 experimental
units.
After eggs hatched in lab, they were added to 360-L
cattle tank mesocosms that closely imitate the small
ephemeral ponds in which these tadpoles develop in nature. Each mesocosm contained 45 Hyla individuals and
30 Rana individuals. Mesocosms were kept in ambient
conditions in an open field in Houston, Texas. One week
prior to the first tadpole additions (2 April), we filled
mesocosms with dechlorinated water and immediately
covered each mesocosm with 60% shade cloth to prevent
external colonization. Five days prior to the first tadpole
introductions (4 April), we added 400 mL concentrated
phytoplankton and zooplankton inoculate and 4 L of
dried leaf litter collected from margins of local ponds.
These additions are aimed to recreate key aspects of natural pond conditions, providing food and habitat structure for the developing tadpoles. After tadpole
hatchlings were added (9 April–21 April), mesocosms
were monitored daily to collect newly emerged Hyla
froglets (hereafter, metamorphs). Because Rana development time is much slower, their emergence was not captured. Metamorphs were weighed in the lab and then
released. The experiment ended 14 September 2018, at
which point emergence rate had declined substantially to
very low levels (only 1–2 metamorphs collected across
all 60 mesocosms each day), so we were confident
we captured the full emergence period for Hyla
(Appendix S1: Fig. S3). At the conclusion of the experiment (18 September–20 September), mesocosms were
emptied and all remaining tadpoles (mostly Rana) were
removed. Tadpoles removed were photographed, measured (head width and SVL), and released. At this point,
22 Hyla (out of 2,700 initially added) and 283 Rana (out
of 1,620 initially added) were collected from the mesocosms. For Hyla, these remaining individuals were
equally distributed across all treatments (v211 = 16.19,
P = 0.13). We recorded the number of remaining Rana
individuals in each mesocosm, but because Rana face
high mortality when emerging, we suspect these
Ecology, Vol. 100, No. 11
measurements are misleading and do not consider them
in our analysis (Appendix S1: Fig. S4, S5).
We used five response variables to quantify the effect
of phenological mean and synchrony on key demographic rates of Hyla: (1) proportional survival (number
of metamorphs collected divided by 45 hatchlings initially added), (2) total biomass export (cumulative mass
of all metamorphs emerged from a mesocosm), (3) mean
per capita mass (the individual masses of all metamorphs from a mesocosm), (4) mean emergence date
(the date of emergence for each individual from a mesocosm), and (5) standard deviation of emergence date.
Together, these five variables give us a picture of per capita and numeric consequences of phenological mean and
synchrony on Hyla populations. The 22 Hyla tadpoles
collected at the end of the experiment were not included
in these analyses because the mass values of tadpoles
and metamorphs are not comparable and these individuals did not have an emergence date. Lacking reliable estimates for four of the five response variables, we chose to
omit Hyla tadpoles collected at the end of the experiment from all analyses.
Statistical analyses
All analyses were performed in the R statistical computing environment (R Development Core Team 2017).
We ran linear and generalized linear mixed models using
the “lme4” package (Bates et al. 2015) to analyze the
independent and interactive effects of variation in Hyla’s
mean hatching date relative to that of the competitor
Rana (categorical predictor with three levels: early, same,
late) and phenological synchrony (categorical predictor
with three levels: high, medium, low) on the five
response variables detailed above. All response variables
were scaled relative to the appropriate single-species controls by subtraction. For example, considering survival,
we subtracted the proportion survival for low-synchrony
control from the proportion survival for each low-synchrony treatment (early, same, and late relative to Rana).
This approach allowed us to partition the effects of phenological synchrony between population and community
scales (i.e., intraspecific vs. interspecific competition).
For all response variables except per capita mass, this
scaling method did not change the qualitative pattern of
the results (e.g., compare Fig. 3 and Appendix S1:
Fig. S6). In the case of per capita mass, we take special
care to interpret the results. All models were tested with
multiple error structures and selected based on fit with
the data, which was normal error structure for all variables. For the standard deviation of emergence-time
model, assumption of equal variances across treatments
was not met, so this model was reformulated in the
“nlme” package to account for unequal variance in phenological synchrony (Pinheiro et al. 2018). For all models, we included spatial block as a random effect and
analyzed significance of fixed effects and their
November 2019
EFFECTS OF PHENOLOGY ON COMPETITION
Article e02826; page 5
interactions with analysis-of-variance tests with the
“car” package (Fox and Weisberg 2011).
RESULTS
Controls—intraspecific competition
Control populations of Hyla (lacking interspecific
competitor Rana) tested the effects of phenological synchrony on five key demographic response variables:
proportion survival, biomass export, mean per capita
mass, mean time to emergence, and standard deviation
of time to emergence. For these populations, proportion survival of Hyla was lowest in low-synchrony populations (58 3%), highest in medium-synchrony
populations (69 3%), and intermediate in high-synchrony populations (62 22%; Fig. 2A). Hyla total
biomass export (i.e., cumulative mass of all Hyla individuals that survived to emergence within a mesocosm)
was similar across synchrony treatments (ranging
from 5,035 1,678 mg at high synchrony to
6,127 775 mg at low synchrony; Fig. 2B). Mean per
capita Hyla body mass decreased as hatching became
more synchronized—individuals from low-synchrony
populations were 237 54 mg, whereas those from
high-synchrony populations were 177 42 mg
(Fig. 2C). Time to emergence increased as hatching
became more synchronized—individuals from low-synchrony populations took on average 33 10 d to
emerge, and individuals from high-synchrony populations took 56 24 d to emerge (Fig. 2D). Synchrony
of timing at hatching was not maintained in the next
phenological stage (measured as the standard deviation
of individuals’ time to emergence). In fact, synchrony
at hatching was reversed at the emergence stage. Populations that hatched highly synchronized had more
variation in emergence (standard deviation of emergence 15.1 5.6 d), whereas populations that hatched
with low synchrony emerged more highly synchronized
(standard deviation of emergence 10.3 0.8 d; Fig. 3,
Fig. 2E).
FIG. 3. Box plot showing emergence timing of individual
Hyla tadpoles for different manipulations of mean and synchrony in hatching timing.
Experimental treatments—interspecific competition
Survival.—The strength of interspecific competition
(i.e., Hyla survival relative to competitor-free control)
was driven by mean hatching date relative to competitor, and the interaction between mean and synchrony,
but not by synchrony independently (Fig. 4A; mean:
v22, 51 = 30.4, P 6%
when arriving early, 49 7% when arriving at the
FIG. 2. Responses of populations of Hyla to experimental manipulations of hatching synchrony. These plots show control
treatments, without interspecific competition. (A) Proportion of tadpoles that survived to emergence, (B) total biomass export
(i.e., cumulative mass of all tadpoles that survived to emergence), (C) average per capita mass of all metamorphs, (D) average number of days from mean hatching time to emergence, (E) standard deviation in time to emergence for all individuals. Points represent
means 1 standard error (from two replicates).
Article e02826; page 6
SHANNON K. CARTER AND VOLKER H. W. RUDOLF
Ecology, Vol. 100, No. 11
FIG. 4. Responses of populations of Hyla to experimental manipulations of mean and synchrony of hatching timing. All points
are scaled by subtraction relative to the control value for a particular synchrony treatment and variable (control baseline represented by dashed black line). (A) Proportion of Hyla tadpoles that survived to emergence, (B) total biomass export (i.e., cumulative
mass of all Hyla tadpoles that survived to emergence), (C) average per capita mass of all Hyla metamorphs, (D) average number of
days from mean hatching time to emergence, (E) standard deviation in time to emergence for all Hyla individuals. Points represent
means 1 standard error (from six replicates).
same time, and 41 16% when arriving late; for medium-synchrony treatments, survival was 75 7% when
arriving early, 58 13% when arriving at the same
time and 37 17% when arriving late). However,
high-synchrony populations followed a different pattern—survival was lowest when Hyla and Rana had
the same mean hatching date (33 16%) and higher
when either species hatched first (59 17% when Hyla
arrived first, 43 18% when Rana arrived first). Synchrony had the strongest effect when competitors
hatched at the same time, with the proportion of survival ranging from 33 16% at high synchrony to
58 13% at medium synchrony. In contrast, synchrony had very little impact on survival when Hyla
hatched late relative to Rana. In these cases, survival
was equally low across the three synchrony levels,
ranging from 37 17% at medium synchrony to
43 18% at high synchrony, suggesting that strong
competition made synchrony less important. Compared with competitor-free controls, Hyla survival was
equal when they hatched before Rana (survival of all
synchrony treatment populations within 6% of the
appropriate single population controls), but survival
was always lower than that of controls when Hyla
hatched at the same time as or after Rana (survival 9–
29% lower than controls for same arrival and 17–32%
lower for late arrival). This suggests that interspecific
competition between Hyla and Rana is negligible when
Hyla hatches first.
Biomass export.—The competitive effect (i.e., Hyla biomass relative to competitor-free controls) depended only
on mean hatching time relative to competitor, but not on
phenological synchrony or the interaction between them
(Fig. 4B; mean: v22, 51 = 21.6, P 28 d) vs. early (59 25 d), but no
additional cost if late (99 22 d). On the other hand,
for low-synchrony populations, hatching earlier or at
the same time as competitor results in the same development time (46 22 d for early, 54 25 d for same),
but there is a cost when hatching late (84 34 d).
Standard deviation of emergence time depended on
mean hatching relative to Rana, hatching synchrony,
and the interaction between synchrony and mean
Article e02826; page 7
(Fig. 4E; mean: v22, 51 = 9.34, P = 0.0094, synchrony:
v22, 51 = 12.4, P = 0.0020, mean * synchrony: v24,
51 = 18.6, P = 0.00096). For medium- and high-synchrony populations, standard deviation of emergence
was hump-shaped: highest when hatching coincided
with interspecific competitor Rana, and lower when
either species hatched first. For Hyla populations that
hatched with low synchrony, standard deviation of
emergence increased as Hyla hatched later relative to
Rana. Across all treatments, synchrony of emergence
was much lower than synchrony in hatching. Although
hatching spanned at most a 13-d window, the average
duration of the emergence period was 98 27 d.
Commonly, emergence distributions had a bimodal
shape, indicating two distinct cohorts of Hyla metamorphs arising from one cohort of Hyla hatchlings
(Appendix S1: Fig. S7).
DISCUSSION
Climate change drives phenological shifts across taxa,
and it is critical that we understand how this temporal
restructuring affects species interactions and, in turn,
natural communities (Parmesan and Yohe 2003, MillerRushing et al. 2010, Yang and Rudolf 2010). Shifts
in first and mean phenological dates are well documented, and recent work has shown that shifts in synchrony (individual variation around these metrics) can
be just as common (CaraDonna et al. 2014, Carter et al.
2018). However, we know little about how both types of
phenological shifts interact to affect species interactions
and natural communities. Using an empirical system, we
found that shifts in phenological synchrony could have
similar or even stronger effects than shifts in mean phenologies. Furthermore, effects of these two aspects of
phenology were often synergistic. Therefore, making
meaningful predictions about how phenological shifts
will disrupt species interactions necessitates broadening our view of phenology to include phenological
synchrony.
Effects of phenological synchrony on intraspecific
competition
The outcomes of intraspecific competition depend on
the abundance of individuals and their per capita composition (Werner and Gilliam 1984). Phenological synchrony can affect both because it affects the density and
size structure of a population at any given point in time
(Rasmussen and Rudolf 2015). We expect low-synchrony
populations to have low density and much variation
between individuals in size and thus competitive ability.
This should lead to asymmetric competition where relatively few individuals can monopolize a limiting
resource. High- synchrony populations should have
higher densities and little variation in body size among
individuals, leading to symmetric competition where
resources are divided more evenly amongst competitively
Article e02826; page 8
SHANNON K. CARTER AND VOLKER H. W. RUDOLF
equal individuals (Henson and Cushing 1996, Rasmussen and Rudolf 2015). Our results support these
expectations. Compared with high-synchrony populations, populations that hatched with low synchrony had
lower survival, but surviving individuals were larger and
developed quickly.
Effects of phenological shifts on interspecific competition
Phenologies play a key role in shaping interspecific
interactions because they define when and for how long
species are present in their environment and able to
interact with other members of the community (Anderson et al. 2015, Kharoub et al. 2018, Renner and Zohner 2018). Considering resource competitors, it is well
known that order of arrival can strongly affect the interaction via size-mediated priority effects (Sutherland and
Karlson 1977, Rasmussen et al. 2014, Rudolf 2018). In
contrast, virtually nothing is known about if or how phenological synchrony within populations can change this
relationship. In the simplest case, population synchrony
could be unimportant. Alternatively, population synchrony could interact additively or synergistically with
differences in species’ mean phenologies. It is difficult to
intuit which case is most likely, and only one study has
tested the outcomes of these concomitant shifts (Rasmussen and Rudolf 2016). Mean hatching time affected
all five attributes of Hyla we measured in our study.
Although this emphasizes the importance of shifts in the
mean timing of phenologies for species interactions, the
effects of population synchrony were often just as
strong, and for three of these attributes, population synchrony modified the effect of changes in mean arrival
time (and vice versa). Only for one response were effects
of mean and synchrony additive.
These interactive effects of population synchrony and
differences in mean phenology between species likely
arise because changing population synchrony alters
competitive asymmetry within populations (intraspecific
effects) and how individuals experience interspecific
competition. As expected with size-mediated priority
effects (Yang and Rudolf 2010, Rasmussen et al. 2014),
survival generally decreased as Hyla arrived later relative
to its competitor. However, the rate of decline differed
based on synchrony: compared with medium- and highsynchrony populations, low-synchrony populations did
slightly worse in the best scenarios (early arrival) but
slightly better in the worst scenarios (late arrival). This
makes intuitive sense, because individuals in highsynchrony populations all experienced either favorable
conditions (reduced interspecific competition with relative early arrival) or unfavorable conditions (increased
interspecific competition with relative late arrival). In
contrast, in low-synchrony populations, only earlyhatching individuals would experience reduced interspecific competition with late arrival of the interspecific
competitor. However, early arrival of the interspecific
competitor should reduce competitive asymmetry within
Ecology, Vol. 100, No. 11
a low-synchrony population by reducing the growth rate
and survival of early-hatching individuals (Morin 1986,
Gimnig et al. 2002, Couret et al. 2014). This would reduce
the negative effect of early hatchlings on later-hatching
conspecifics and could thereby at least partially compensate for the negative effect of interspecific competition.
This mechanism is consistent with the concurrent
changes in total biomass. Qualitatively, biomass followed a very similar pattern to survival, but unlike survival, there was no significant interaction between mean
and synchrony treatments for biomass. Even though we
did not detect a significant effect of synchrony on biomass, this is likely because effects of population synchrony on individual mass and survival counteracted
each other. At the population scale, compensatory
dynamics between survival and individual mass led to
relatively uniform biomass across population synchrony
treatments. In a community context, compensatory
dynamics (i.e., when survival was low, individuals tended
to be larger) buffered biomass across different ecological
contexts, thereby reducing differences between synchrony treatments. These complex interactions between
species’ mean phenologies and phenological synchrony
within populations indicate that predicting the outcomes
of mean phenological shifts on species interactions
requires consideration of distribution of phenologies
within populations.
Overall, the results indicate maintenance of low phenological synchrony within populations as a bet-hedging
strategy. Bet-hedging life-history strategies increase fitness in unpredictable environments (Tarazona et al.
2017, Rocha et al. 2018, Shima et al. 2018), and our
results show individual variation in hatching could buffer survival across good and bad conditions. By “putting
all their eggs in one basket,” highly synchronized populations run a great risk of mistiming because all individuals are affected. In good scenarios, survival was highest,
but when populations arrived at the same time or late
relative to their competitor, all individuals faced strong
competition and survival was low. Similarly, when highly
synchronized populations mistime events (e.g., migratory birds arriving before their food, alpine flowers
blooming before a snowfall, insect swarms emerging
before spring green up), fitness costs are high (Both
et al. 2006, Inouye 2008, Mayor et al. 2017).
The relationships of phenologies across life stages
Organisms go through a series of life-history stages
during a year, but we rarely pay attention to whether
and how the phenological patterns are preserved or
change across life-history events and what the underlying mechanisms are. In our study, we found that phenological patterns were not preserved across stages
(hatching vs. emergence), but instead changed: differences in intra- and interspecific competition caused by
differences in hatching phenology carried over to affect
the phenology of the next ontogenetic stage, emergence.
November 2019
EFFECTS OF PHENOLOGY ON COMPETITION
In cases without interspecific competition, we saw a
complete reversal of synchrony from hatching to emergence: populations that hatched with low synchrony
emerged with high synchrony and vice versa. We attribute this to strong size-mediated priority effects, which
gave early-arriving individuals an advantage in lowsynchrony populations, generated a bias in survival, and
led to synchronous and early-emergence phenology.
High hatching synchrony shifted to low-emergence synchrony because high intraspecific competition prolonged
the interaction period and slowed individuals’ growth.
Adding interspecific competition complicated this by
modifying the drivers—density and per capita differences in size—that caused these shifts. When the interspecific competitor arrived early, density/competition
was higher and the advantage of early arrival was
reduced. In general, interspecific competition increased
mean and variation in emergence phenology, but the
magnitude of these changes depended on initial phenology conditions (i.e., at hatching). Together, this provides
clear experimental evidence that not only can phenological shifts affect species interactions, but species interactions can in turn drive phenological shifts.
This feedback between species interactions and phenology is likely to be seen in systems where competition
is high and organisms’ development rates are plastic.
Because strong competition drove phenological shifts
across ontogeny, we only expect it to happen when
resources are limited to some extent. Further, it requires
individuals to be plastic in their development rates. Consistent with many species that utilize ephemeral habitats,
emergence timing was highly plastic (ranging from 22 to
152 d), which strengthened the advantage of early individuals (Newman 1992, Blanckenhorn 1998, Denver
et al. 1998). In more stable environments, development
times are generally less flexible or even fixed, and in
these cases, we would not expect phenological shifts
across ontogeny to be as strong (DeWitt et al. 1998). It
would be possible to test whether competition drives
phenological shifts in other natural populations without
conducting manipulative experiments by measuring the
phenological synchrony of natural populations at different phenological stages. It remains largely unknown
what mechanisms determine phenology (either mean or
synchrony) across ontogenetic stages and years. Data on
phenological synchrony across ontogenetic stages could
help determine these mechanisms and therefore predict
which species are likely to shift.
CONCLUSIONS
Phenological shifts are a well-documented response to
climate change, and it is time to start linking these patterns to expected impacts in natural communities. We
show that this requires expanding our typical treatment
of phenology to include not just first or mean events,
but also variation around these metrics within populations. Changes in phenological synchrony within
Article e02826; page 9
populations are just as likely to alter ecological interactions as changes in phenological mean, and the effects
are likely to be synergistic. Importantly, our study also
reveals a feedback loop between phenology and ecological interactions: Shifts in phenology can alter the mechanisms driving the outcome of interactions, but the same
changes in mechanisms and outcome of interactions can
in turn alter phenological patterns in the next life stage.
Together, this highlights the need to integrate multiple
aspects of phenological patterns with species interactions to understand and predict the effect of phenological shifts on natural communities.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Thanks to Tom E. X. Miller, Bene Bachelot, Patrick Clay,
Marion Donald, Joey Neale, and Josh Fowler for providing comments on previous versions of the manuscript. We thank Lauren
Eveland and Ally Simoni for help maintaining eggs in the lab,
setting up the mesocosms, and collecting data. Thanks to Rice
University BioSciences Department for fellowship funding to
SKC. The work was supported by National Science Foundation
grant DEB-1655626 to VHWR. Study organisms were collected
under Scientific Research Permit SPR-0409-042 issued by Texas
Parks & Wildlife, and all experimental work was conducted in
accordance with IACUC protocol A18090501.
LITERATURE CITED
Alford, R. A., and H. M. Wilbur. 1985. Priority effects in experimental pond communities: competition between Bufo and
Rana. Ecology 66:1097–1105.
Anderson, T. L., D. J. Hocking, C. A. Conner, J. E. Earl, E. B.
Harper, M. S. Osbourn, W. E. Peterman, T. A. G. Rittenhouse, and R. D. Semlitsch. 2015. Abundance and phenology patterns of two pond-breeding salamanders determine
species interactions in natural populations. Oecologia
177:761–773.
Bates, D., M. Maechler, B. Bolker, and S. Walker. 2015. Fitting
linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical
Software 67:1–48.
Blanckenhorn, W. U. 1998. Adaptive phenotypic plasticity in
growth, development, and body size in the yellow dung fly.
Evolution 52:1394–1407.
Both, C., S. Bouwhuis, C. M. Lessells, and M. E. Visser. 2006.
Climate change and population declines in a long-distance
migratory bird. Nature 441:81–83.
Bury, R. B. 1999. A historical perspective and critique of the
declining amphibian crisis. Wildlife Society Bulletin 27:1064–
1068.
CaraDonna, P. J., A. M. Iler, and D. W. Inouye. 2014. Shifts in
flowering phenology reshape a subalpine plant community.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111:4916–
4921.
Carter, S. K., D. Saenz, and V. H. W. Rudolf. 2018. Shifts in
phenological distributions reshape interaction potential in
natural communities. Ecology Letters 21:1143–1151.
Couret, J., E. Dotson, and M. Q. Benedict. 2014. Temperature,
larval diet, and density effects on development rate and survival of Aedes aegypti (Diptera: Culicidae). PLoS ONE 9:
e87468.
Denver, R. J., N. Mirhadi, and M. Phillips. 1998. Adaptive plasticity in amphibian metamorphosis: response of Scaphiopus hammondii tadpoles to habitat desiccation. Ecology 79:1859–1872.
Article e02826; page 10
SHANNON K. CARTER AND VOLKER H. W. RUDOLF
DeWitt, T. J., A. Sih, and D. S. Wilson. 1998. Costs and limits
of phenotypic plasticity. Trends in Ecology and Evolution
13:77–81.
Fox, J., and S. Weisberg. 2011. An R companion to applied
regression. Second edition. Sage Publications, Thousand
Oaks, California, USA.
Fukami, T. 2010. Community assembly dynamics in space.
Pages 45–54 in H. A. Verhoef, and P. J. Morin, editors. Community ecology: processes, models, and applications. Oxford
University Press, Oxford, UK.
Gimnig, J. E., M. Ombok, S. Otieno, M. G. Kaufman, J. M.
Vulule, and E. D. Walker. 2002. Density-dependent development of Anopheles gambiae (Diptera: Culicidae) larvae in
artificial habitats. Journal of Medical Entomology 39:162–
172.
Henson, S. M., and J. M. Cushing. 1996. Hierarchical models
of intra-specific competition: scramble versus contest. Journal of Mathematical Biology 34:755–772.
Inouye, D. W. 2008. Effects of climate change on phenology,
frost damage, and floral abundance of montane wildflowers.
Ecology 89:353–362.
Kharoub, H., J. Ehrlen, A. Gelman, K. Bolmgren, J. Allen, S. Travers, and E. Wolkovich. 2018. Global shifts in the phenological
synchrony of species interactions over recent decades. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115:5211–5216.
Koenig, W. D., J. M. H. Knops, W. J. Carmen, and I. S. Pearse.
2015. What drives masting? The phenological synchrony
hypothesis. Ecology 96:184–192.
Loe, L. E., C. Bonenfant, A. Mysterud, J.-M. Gaillard, R.
Langvatn, F. Klein, C. Calenge, T. Ergon, N. Pettorelli, and
N. C. Stenseth. 2005. Climate predictability and breeding
phenology in red deer: timing and synchrony of rutting and
calving in Norway and France. Journal of Animal Ecology
74:579–588.
Mayor, S. J., R. P. Guralnick, M. W. Tingley, J. Otegui, J. C.
Withey, S. C. Elmendorf, M. E. Andrew, S. Leyk, I. S. Pearse,
and D. C. Schneider. 2017. Increasing phenological asynchrony between spring green-up and arrival of migratory
birds. Scientific Reports 7:1902.
Menzel, A. et al. 2006. European phenological response to climate change matches the warming pattern. Global Change
Biology 12:1969–1976.
Miller-Rushing, A. J., T. T. Høye, D. W. Inouye, and E. Post. 2010.
The effects of phenological mismatches on demography. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 365:3177–3186.
Moore, J. A. 1939. Temperature tolerance and rates of development in the eggs of amphibia. Ecology 20:459–478.
Morin, P. J. 1986. Interactions between intraspecific competition and predation in an amphibian predator–prey system.
Ecology 67:713–720.
Newman, R. A. 1992. Adaptive plasticity in amphibian metamorphosis. BioScience 42:671–678.
Parmesan, C. 2006. Ecological and evolutionary responses to
recent climate change. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution,
and Systematics 37:637–669.
Parmesan, C. 2007. Influences of species, latitudes and methodologies on estimates of phenological response to global
warming. Global Change Biology 13:1860–1872.
Parmesan, C., and G. Yohe. 2003. A globally coherent fingerprint of climate change impacts across natural systems. Nature 421:37–42.
Pinheiro, J., D. Bates, S. DebRoy, D. Sarkar, and R Core Team.
2018. nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R
package version 3.1-137.
R Development Core Team. 2017. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical
Computing, Vienna, Austria. www.r-project.org
Ecology, Vol. 100, No. 11
Rasmussen, N. L., and V. H. W. Rudolf. 2015. Phenological synchronization drives demographic rates of populations. Ecology 96:1754–1760.
Rasmussen, N. L., and V. H. W. Rudolf. 2016. Individual and
combined effects of two types of phenological shifts on
predator-prey interactions. Ecology 97:3414–3421.
Rasmussen, N. L., B. G. Van Allen, and V. H. W. Rudolf. 2014.
Linking phenological shifts to species interactions through
size-mediated priority effects. Journal of Animal Ecology
83:1206–1215.
Renner, S. S., and C. M. Zohner. 2018. Climate change and phenological mismatch in trophic interactions among plants,
insects, and vertebrates. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 49:165–182.
Rocha, O. J., C. G
omez, J. L. Hamrick, D. W. Trapnell, P. E.
Smouse, and G. Macaya. 2018. Reproductive consequences
of variation in flowering phenology in the dry forest tree
Enterolobium cyclocarpum in Guanacaste, Costa Rica. American Journal of Botany 105:2037–2050.
Root, T. L., J. T. Price, K. R. Hall, S. H. Schneider, C. Rosenzweig, and J. A. Pounds. 2003. Fingerprints of global warming on wild animals and plants. Nature 421:57–60.
Rudolf, V. H. W. 2018. Nonlinear effects of phenological shifts
link interannual variation to species interactions. Journal of
Animal Ecology 87:1395–1406.
Rudolf, V. H. W., and S. McCrory. 2018. Resource limitation
alters effects of phenological shifts on inter-specific competition. Oecologia 188:515–523.
Rudolf, V. H. W., and N. L. Rasmussen. 2013. Population structure determines functional differences among species and
ecosystem processes. Nature Communications 4:2318.
Rudolf, V. H. W., and M. Singh. 2013. Disentangling climate
change effects on species interactions: effects of temperature, phenological shifts, and body size. Oecologia 173:1043–
1052.
Shima, J. S., E. G. Noonburg, S. E. Swearer, S. H. Alonzo, and
C. W. Osenberg. 2018. Born at the right time? A conceptual
framework linking reproduction, development, and settlement in reef fish. Ecology 99:116–126.
Stuart, S. N., J. S. Chanson, N. A. Cox, B. E. Young, A. S. L.
Rodrigues, D. L. Fischman, and R. W. Waller. 2004. Status
and trends of amphibian declines and extinctions worldwide.
Science 306:1783–1786.
Sutherland, J. P., and R. H. Karlson. 1977. Development and
stability of the fouling community at Beaufort, North Carolina. Ecological Monographs 47:425–446.
Tarazona, E., E. M. Garcıa-Roger, and M. J. Carmona. 2017.
Experimental evolution of bet hedging in rotifer diapause
traits as a response to environmental unpredictability. Oikos
126:1162–1172.
Taylor, S. G. 2008. Climate warming causes phenological shift
in Pink Salmon, Oncorhynchus gorbuscha, behavior at Auke
Creek, Alaska. Global Change Biology 14:229–235.
Thackeray, S. J. et al. 2016. Phenological sensitivity to climate
across taxa and trophic levels. Nature 535:241–245.
Tilman, D. 1988. Plant strategies and the dynamics and structure of plant communities. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, USA.
Todd, B. D., D. E. Scott, J. H. K. Pechmann, and J. W. Gibbons.
2010. Climate change correlates with rapid delays and
advancements in reproductive timing in an amphibian community. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B
278:2191–2197.
Vitasse, Y., C. Signarbieux, and Y. H. Fu. 2018. Global warming leads to more uniform spring phenology across elevations.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115:1004–
1008.
November 2019
EFFECTS OF PHENOLOGY ON COMPETITION
Werner, E. E., and J. F. Gilliam. 1984. The ontogenetic niche
and species interactions in size-structured populations.
Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15:393–425.
Wilbur, H. M., V. H. W. Rudolf, A. E. J. M. McNamara, and E.
M. C. Whitlock. 2006. Life-history evolution in uncertain
environments: bet hedging in time. American Naturalist
168:398–411.
Article e02826; page 11
Wolkovich, E. M., B. I. Cook, K. K. McLauchlan, and T. J.
Davies. 2014. Temporal ecology in the Anthropocene. Ecology Letters 17:1365–1379.
Yang, L. H., and V. H. W. Rudolf. 2010. Phenology, ontogeny
and the effects of climate change on the timing of species
interactions. Ecology Letters 13:1–10.
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of this article at http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/
10.1002/ecy.2826/suppinfo
DATA AVAILABILITY
Data and scripts are available from the Dryad Data Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.69325t8
Name: ___________________________________________ Date: _______________________
Current Research Topics #4
Use the following questions to help you work through the posted journal article. You will then
complete the write-up assignment (on page 2 of this document). You will submit this sheet with
your answers typed along with your write up.
1. Abstract
a. Define phenological shifts (You will need to look this information up outside of the
article. Cite your source.)
b. In your own words, what is meant by the term phenological mean?
c. In your own words, what is meant by the term population phenological synchrony?
d. What do the authors state is the purpose or hypothesis that they are testing in the study?
(Write down the exact sentence where the authors describe what they are doing in the
study. Include quotation marks.)
e. What is the current gap in knowledge that the authors are trying to address with their
study?
f. What experimental tools or measurements did the authors use in their methods to test
their hypothesis.
g. What do the authors say are the major conclusions or findings of the study? Include
quotation marks where appropriate.
h. What are the significant contributions of this study to the scientific literature at large?
Name: ___________________________________________ Date: _______________________
2. Introduction
a. What is the big picture problem that led the authors to study this particular research
question? (Impact/Relevance…Why should the reader care about the study?)
b. What have previous studies demonstrated in relation to the big picture problem?
c. What are the specific gaps in the current knowledge that the authors are trying to
address?
d. What is the experimental organism/experimental system being used? What are its
advantages and disadvantages? (Why did the authors choose this system?)
e. What do the authors say are the major conclusions or findings of the study? Include
quotation marks and page numbers where appropriate.
f. Figure 1
i) What is the purpose of this figure?
ii) What is phenological mean?
iii) What is synchrony?
iv) Select a single row or column and write a scientific question with a testable
hypothesis using this experimental set up.
v) Consider:
• What variable would you measure?
• Recall population dynamics we have discussed
• What controls would you need?
3. Results
a. Figure 2
i) What is the experimental question being asked?
ii) Describe the results for part each part (A-E)
iii) What, if anything, can we conclude from this data?
iv) How do the results of this figure relate to what you’ve learned about intraspecific
population
v) What is a question that you would like to follow up on based on this data? (the next
experiment)
b. Figure 3
i) What is the experimental question being asked?
ii) Describe the results for part each part (early, same, late, control).
iii) What, if anything, can we conclude from this data?
Name: ___________________________________________ Date: _______________________
c. Figure 4a: Hyla Survival
• Phenological mean:
• Trend for low and medium synchrony?
• High synchrony pattern?
• Effect of synchrony:
• For which mean Hyla hatching does synchrony have the greatest impact on
survival?
• For which…does synchrony have the least impact on survival?
• Effect of interspecific competition:
• Relative to controls (no Rana)
d. Figure 4b: Biomass Export
• Phenological mean:
• Trend for low and medium synchrony?
• High synchrony pattern?
• Effect of synchrony:
• For which mean Hyla hatching does synchrony have the greatest impact on
biomass export?
• For which…does synchrony have the least impact on biomass export?
• Effect of interspecific competition:
• Relative to controls (no Rana)
e. Figure 4c: Per capita Biomass
• Phenological mean:
• Low synchrony trend?
• Medium synchrony trend?
• High synchrony trend?
• Effect of synchrony:
• What does the data show with regard to the relationship of synchrony and per
capita mass?
• Effect of interspecific competition:
• Relative to controls (no Rana)
f. Figure 4d: Time to Emergence
• Phenological mean:
• Low synchrony trend?
• Medium synchrony trend?
• High synchrony trend?
• Effect of synchrony:
• What is notable about the variation in days to emergence?
• Effect of interspecific competition:
• Relative to controls (no Rana)
Name: ___________________________________________ Date: _______________________
g. Figure 4d: Emergence Synchrony
• Phenological mean:
• Low synchrony trend?
• Trend for medium and high synchrony?
• Effect of synchrony:
• For which mean Hyla hatching does synchrony have the greatest impact on per
capita mass?
• For which…does synchrony have the least impact on per capita mass?
• Effect of interspecific competition:
• Relative to controls (no Rana)
4. Discussion
a. What are the main conclusions of this study?
b. What scientific questions still remain in this field of study that can build off of this work?

Purchase answer to see full
attachment

SOLUTION: Carnegie Mellon University What Is Meant by The Term Phenological Mean Questions

Calculate your order
Pages (275 words)
Standard price: $0.00
Client Reviews
4.9
Sitejabber
4.6
Trustpilot
4.8
Our Guarantees
100% Confidentiality
All your data is secure and will never be disclosed to third parties. Your essay or assignment is treated as your intellectual property and can never be shared or provided as a sample to aspiring customers.
Original Writing
We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
Timely Delivery
You will never have to worry about deadlines – 98% of our assignments are completed on time.
Money Back
We give refunds anytime you feel the work did not meet your expectations. However, we have not refunded any papers in the last 6 months as our team keeps improving their quality and customer service.

Calculate the price of your order

You will get a personal manager and a discount.
We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
Total price:
$0.00
Power up Your Academic Success with the
Team of writers and tutors. We are here for you.
Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.